Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 35
Filtrar
Más filtros










Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Iperception ; 15(2): 20416695241238692, 2024.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38577221

RESUMEN

In a spatial Stroop task, eye-gaze targets produce a reversed congruency effect (RCE) with faster responses when gaze direction and location are incongruent than congruent. On the other hand, non-social directional targets (e.g., arrows) elicit a spatial Stroop effect (SSE). The present study examined whether other social stimuli, such as head orientation, trigger the RCE. Participants judged the target direction of the head or the gaze while ignoring its location. While the gaze target replicated the RCE, the head target produced the SSE. Moreover, the head target facilitated the overall responses relative to the gaze target. These results suggest that the head, a salient directional feature, overrides the social significance. The RCE may be specific to gaze stimuli, not to social stimuli in general. The head and gaze information differentially affect our attentional mechanisms and enable us to bring about smooth social interactions.

2.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; : 17470218241232981, 2024 Feb 26.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38320865

RESUMEN

Numerous studies have shown that the gaze of others produces a special attentional process, such as the eye contact effect or joint attention. This study investigated the attentional process triggered by various types of gaze stimuli (i.e., human, cat, fish, koala, and robot gaze). A total of 300 university students participated in five experiments. They performed a spatial Stroop task in which five types of gaze stimuli were presented as targets. Participants were asked to judge the direction of the target (left or right) irrespective of its location (left or right). The results showed that the social gaze targets (i.e., human and cat gaze) produced a reversed congruency effect. In contrast to the social gaze targets, the non-social gaze (i.e., fish and robot) target did not produce the reversed congruency effect (Experiments 2, 2B, 3, and 4). These results suggest that attention to the gaze of socially communicable beings (i.e., humans and cats) is responsible for the reversed congruency effect. Our findings support the notion that the theory of mind or social interaction plays an important role in producing specific attentional processes in response to gaze stimuli.

3.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; : 17470218231203187, 2023 Oct 17.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37705449

RESUMEN

In the spatial Stroop task, an arrow target produces a spatial Stroop effect, whereas a gaze target elicits a reversed congruency effect. The reversed congruency effect has been explained by the unique attentional mechanisms of eye gaze. However, recent studies have shown that not only gaze but arrow targets produced a reversed congruency effect when embedded in a complex background. The present study investigated whether non-gaze targets produce a reversed congruency effect. In Experiments 1 and 2, we used the tongue, which is not commonly used to indicate spatial directions in daily life, as a target in the spatial Stroop task, in addition to the conventional gaze and arrows. In Experiment 3, we used arrow stimuli embedded in a complex background as a target. Participants judged the left/right direction of the target presented in the left or right visual field. Although arrow and gaze targets replicated previous findings (spatial Stroop and reversed congruency effect, respectively), the tongue target produced a reversed congruency effect (Experiments 1 and 2). The spatial Stroop effect of arrow targets disappeared when they were in a complex background (Experiment 3). These results are inconsistent with previous accounts emphasising the unique status of eye gaze. We propose that temporal decay of the location code and response inhibition are responsible for the reversal of spatial interference.

4.
Laterality ; 28(4-6): 357-376, 2023.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37608647

RESUMEN

People tend to deviate to the right when walking through a narrow aperture (e.g., a doorway), resulting in a rightward bias in collisions. This study examines the effects of smartphone use on rightward collisions while walking. When pedestrians walk through a narrow aperture, they usually head straight to the perceived centre of the aperture, which is shifted slightly to the right, without updating the estimates. The rightward shift of the perceived centre is attributable to the rightward attentional shift in the extrapersonal space. Pedestrians using smartphones tend to fixate on the phone most of the time and thus tend not to look at their surroundings (i.e., extrapersonal space). Therefore, we predict that smartphone use will reduce rightward collisions. To test this prediction, we used a narrow-doorway task in which participants walked through a narrow doorway either with or without a smartphone. The participants with smartphones used them to perform either verbal or spatial tasks. The number of rightward collisions decreased when the participants used smartphones. The type of task had no effect on the lateral collision biases. These results were interpreted in terms of lateral attentional bias in peripersonal and extrapersonal spaces.

5.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 30(3): 974-982, 2023 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36307577

RESUMEN

In a spatial Stroop task, the eye-gaze target produces the reversed congruency effect-responses become shorter when the gaze direction and its location are incongruent than when they are congruent. The present study examined the face inversion effect on the gaze spatial Stroop task to clarify whether the holistic face processing or part-based processing of the eyes is responsible for the reversed congruency effect. In Experiment 1, participants judged the gaze direction of the upright or inverted face with a neutral expression presented either in the left or right visual field. In Experiment 2, we examined whether face inversion interacted with facial expressions (i.e., angry, happy, neutral, and sad). Face inversion disrupted holistic face processing, slowing down the overall performance relative to the performance with upright faces. However, face inversion did not affect the reversed congruency effect. These results further support the parts-based processing account and suggest that while faces are processed holistically, the reversed congruency effect, relying on the extracted local features (i.e., eyes), may be processed in a part-based manner.


Asunto(s)
Ira , Fijación Ocular , Humanos , Ojo , Felicidad , Campos Visuales , Expresión Facial
6.
Laterality ; 27(1): 6-20, 2022 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34088246

RESUMEN

People tend to show the left cheek to broadly express emotions while they tend to show the right cheek to hide emotions because emotions were expressed more on the left than on the right side of the face. The present study investigated the level of awareness on the left- and right-cheek poses using the method of structural knowledge attributions. When asked to broadly express emotions for a family portrait, right-handed participants were more likely to show the left cheek than the right. On the other hand, when asked to conceal emotions to show a calm and reassuring attitude as a scientist, they were more likely to show the right cheek. After the posing session, participants selected the conscious level of their knowledge about posing from five categories: Random, intuition, familiarity, recollection, and rules. Most participants rated their knowledge as unconscious (i.e., either as random, intuition, or familiarity). The choice of the conscious level did not differ across posing orientations and posing instructions. These results suggest that although people do not have an acute awareness of their lateral posing preference, they reliably show one side of their faces to express or hide emotions.


Asunto(s)
Expresión Facial , Lateralidad Funcional , Mejilla , Emociones , Mano , Humanos
7.
Cogn Emot ; 35(6): 1163-1174, 2021 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34078237

RESUMEN

Socially anxious people have a malfunction in attentional systems. However, it is uncertain whether the malfunction of the attentional system is a domain-specific process to social stimuli or a domain-general process to non-social stimuli. Therefore, we investigated the effects of social anxiety on the domain specificity of the attentional process using a spatial Stroop paradigm. We conducted two identical experiments with a total of 153 university students including men and women (61 students in Experiment 1 and 92 students in Experiment 2), in which the levels of social anxiety were assessed using specific instruments. The results showed that social anxiety scores were negatively correlated with the reversed spatial Stroop effect for social stimuli, but not for non-social stimuli (Experiment 1). The findings of the first experiment were successfully replicated in Experiment 2. Our results suggested that the malfunction of the attentional system is a domain-specific process to socially threatening stimuli in socially anxious individuals.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Expresión Facial , Ansiedad , Trastornos de Ansiedad , Miedo , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
8.
PLoS One ; 15(2): e0229523, 2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32069331

RESUMEN

[This corrects the article DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0227513.].

9.
PLoS One ; 15(1): e0227513, 2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31935264

RESUMEN

Images of European female and male faces were digitally processed to generate spatial frequency (SF) filtered images containing only a narrow band of visual information within the Fourier spectrum. The original unfiltered images and four SF filtered images (low, medium-low, medium-high and high) were then paired in trials that kept constant SF band and face gender and participants made a forced-choice decision about the more attractive among the two faces. In this way, we aimed at identifying those specific SF bands where forced-choice preferences corresponded best to forced-choice judgements made when viewing the natural, broadband, facial images. We found that aesthetic preferences dissociated across SFs and face gender, but similarly for participants from Asia (Japan) and Europe (Norway). Specifically, preferences when viewing SF filtered images were best related to the preference with the broadband face images when viewing the highest filtering band for the female faces (about 48-77 cycles per face). In contrast, for the male faces, the medium-low SF band (about 11-19 cpf) related best to choices made with the natural facial images. Eye tracking provided converging evidence for the above, gender-related, SF dissociations. We suggest greater aesthetic relevance of the mobile and communicative parts for the female face and, conversely, of the rigid, structural, parts for the male face for facial aesthetics.


Asunto(s)
Belleza , Juicio , Adulto , Cara , Reconocimiento Facial , Femenino , Humanos , Japón , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Noruega , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Procesamiento de Señales Asistido por Computador , Percepción Visual , Adulto Joven
10.
Laterality ; 24(6): 678-696, 2019 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30835641

RESUMEN

Cheater detection, which is a prerequisite for the evolution of social cooperation, has been successfully simulated in laboratory settings. However, the process has not been perfect because the detection rate has usually been just above chance. The present study investigated the role of lateral posing biases and emotional expressions in displaying trustworthiness, which plays a crucial role in cheater detection. Participants (N = 30 and 28 in Experiments 1 and 2, respectively) observed facial photographs of cheaters and cooperators in an economic game and evaluated their facial expressions in terms of emotional valence and arousal. The models in the photographs had turned their left or right cheek to the camera to display their trustworthiness in the economic game. The results indicated that cheaters showing their left cheek were rated as more emotionally positive than cheaters showing their right cheek. This lateral difference was not observed for cooperators. A left cheek advantage in emotional arousal was found for both cheaters and cooperators. These results suggest that cheaters use a fake smile on the emotional side of their face (i.e., the left) to conceal their uncooperative attitude.


Asunto(s)
Decepción , Emociones/fisiología , Expresión Facial , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Sonrisa/psicología , Confianza/psicología , Nivel de Alerta , Juegos Experimentales , Humanos , Detección de Mentiras , Masculino , Adulto Joven
11.
Laterality ; 24(1): 56-64, 2019 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29676220

RESUMEN

Laboratory studies have shown that people tend to show the left side of their face when asked to broadly express emotions, while they tend to show the right side when asked to hide emotions. Because emotions are expressed more intensely in the left side of the face, it is hypothesized that an individual's intention to express or hide emotions biases the direction of lateral facial poses. The present study tested this hypothesis using photographic portraits of individuals experiencing emotional events in a naturalistic setting: the reception of medals in Brazilian jiu-jitsu competitions. Portrait photographs of Brazilian jiu-jitsu competitors were sourced online (N = 460) and were rated by two independent raters in terms of posing direction, emotional expression, and medal colour. Gold and silver medallists showed their left cheeks to the camera for commemorative photographs taken immediately after the medal ceremony. Positive emotions were expressed more often for gold medallists than silver ones. The left-cheek posing bias observed in the present study supports the hypothesis that the intended purpose of expressing or hiding emotions determines the direction of lateral posing biases, and extends the laboratory findings to situations in the real world.


Asunto(s)
Sesgo , Expresión Facial , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Emociones , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Artes Marciales , Fotograbar
12.
Neurobiol Aging ; 73: 1-8, 2019 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30300745

RESUMEN

Behavioral studies suggest that older adults may be less adept than younger adults at remembering information contradicting their first impressions about others' trustworthiness. To identify the neural bases associated with such age-related differences, we measured the brain activity of older and younger participants using functional magnetic resonance imaging while they processed feedback on whether their initial trustworthiness impressions of stimulus persons, whose true trustworthiness had been predetermined, were right or wrong. Of special interest was the activation in mentalizing- (e.g., medial prefrontal cortex) and reward-related brain regions (e.g., striatum), which are known to be involved in impression formation and feedback learning, respectively. The reduction in the striatal responses to impression-contradicting versus impression-confirming feedback was greater in older than in younger participants. The activation of some mentalizing-related regions (medial prefrontal cortex and precuneus) was lower in older than younger participants; however, it was not modulated by impression-feedback congruency. The results suggest that age-related differences in the striatum engagement may underlie older adults' inefficiency in learning impression-incongruent information about others' trustworthiness.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/fisiología , Envejecimiento/psicología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Cara/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Mentalización/fisiología , Recompensa , Confianza , Adulto , Anciano , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Retroalimentación Psicológica , Femenino , Humanos , Juicio/fisiología , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Adulto Joven
13.
PLoS One ; 13(8): e0201603, 2018.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30071065

RESUMEN

In an internet-based, forced-choice, test of the 'face race lightness illusion', the majority of respondents, regardless of their ethnicity, reported perceiving the African face as darker in skin tone than the European face, despite the mean luminance, contrast and numbers of pixels of the images were identical. In the laboratory, using eye tracking, it was found that eye fixations were distributed differently on the African face and European face, so that gaze dwelled relatively longer onto the locally brighter regions of the African face and, in turn, mean pupil diameters were smaller than for the European face. There was no relationship between pupils' size and implicit social attitude (IAT) scores. In another experiment, the faces were presented either tachistoscopically (140 ms) or longer (2500 ms) so that, when gaze was prevented from looking directly at the faces in the former condition, the tendency to report the African face as "dark" disappeared, but it was present when gaze was free to move for just a few seconds. We conclude that the presence of the illusion depends on oculomotor behavior and we also propose a novel account based on a predictive strategy of sensory acquisition. Specifically, by differentially directing gaze towards to facial regions that are locally different in luminance, the resulting changes in retinal illuminance yield respectively darker or brighter percepts while attending to each face, hence minimizing the mismatch between visual input and the learned perceptual prototypes of ethnic categories.


Asunto(s)
Cara/fisiología , Fenómenos Fisiológicos Oculares , Pupila/fisiología , Adulto , Etnicidad , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
14.
J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry ; 61: 32-37, 2018 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29890335

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES: Individuals with social anxiety have various types of deficiencies in emotional processing. Diversity of deficiencies may imply that socially anxious individuals have malfunctions in fundamental parts of emotional processing. Therefore, we hypothesized that social anxiety contributes to deficiencies in building on the metaphorical relationship between emotional experience and brightness. METHODS: We conducted a judgment task of valences of faces with manipulated clothing brightness (bright or dark). RESULTS: A congruency effect between the emotional valence and clothing brightness was observed in participants with low social anxiety. However, this pattern was not found in participants with high social anxiety. The results suggested that a deficiency in metaphorical associations leads to maladaptive emotional processing in individuals with social anxiety. LIMITATIONS: Our findings cannot be directly generalized to clinical populations. Such populations should be tested in the future studies. CONCLUSIONS: We may expand Lakoff and Johnson's (1999) conceptual metaphor theory by showing the relationships between social anxiety and malfunction in metaphorical processing. Malfunctions in metaphorical processing could lead to various types of psychological disorders which have deficiencies in emotional processing.


Asunto(s)
Ansiedad/fisiopatología , Vestuario , Percepción de Color/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Reconocimiento Facial/fisiología , Metáfora , Percepción Social , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
15.
Laterality ; 23(2): 209-227, 2018 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28707557

RESUMEN

Our cognitive mechanisms are designed to detect cheaters in social exchanges. However, cheater detection can be thwarted by a posed smile, which cheaters display with greater emotional intensity than cooperators. The present study investigated the role of hemifacial asymmetries in the perception of trustworthiness using face photographs with left and right cheek poses. Participants (N = 170) observed face photographs of cheaters and cooperators in an economic game. In the photographs, models expressed happiness or anger and turned slightly to the left or right to show their left or right cheeks to the camera. When the models expressed anger on their faces, cheaters showing the right cheek were rated as less trustworthy than cooperators (irrespective of cheeks shown) and cheaters showing the left cheek. When the models expressed happiness, trustworthiness ratings increased and did not differ between cheaters and cooperators, and no substantial asymmetries were observed. These patterns were replicated even when the face photographs were mirror-reversed. These results suggest that a cheater's fake smile conceals an uncooperative attitude that is displayed in the right hemiface, ultimately disguising cheater detection.


Asunto(s)
Mejilla , Conducta Cooperativa , Expresión Facial , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Relaciones Interpersonales , Percepción/fisiología , Adolescente , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Estimulación Luminosa , Adulto Joven
16.
Front Psychol ; 7: 611, 2016.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27199844

RESUMEN

It is known that threatening stimuli increase emotional arousal, resulting in overestimating the subjective experience of passing time. Moreover, facial expressions and gaze direction interact to create socially threatening situations in people with social anxiety. The present study investigated the effect of social anxiety on the perceived duration of observing emotional faces with a direct or an averted gaze. Participants were divided into high, medium and low social anxiety groups based on social anxiety inventory scores. Participants then performed a temporal bisection task. Participants with high social anxiety provided larger overestimates for neutral faces with an averted gaze than those with low social anxiety in the second half of the task, whereas these differences were not found for angry face with direct and averted gaze. These results suggest that people with social anxiety perceive the duration of threatening situations as being longer than true durations based on objectively measured time.

17.
Evol Psychol ; 13(3): 1474704915600567, 2015 Sep 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37924180

RESUMEN

While smiling enhances women's facial attractiveness, the findings are inconclusive for men. The present study investigated the effect of smiling on male facial attractiveness for short- and long-term prospective partners using East Asian and European samples. In Experiment 1 (N = 218), where female participants rated male facial attractiveness, the facilitative effect of smiling was present when judging long-term partners but absent for short-term partners. This pattern was observed for East Asians as well as for Europeans. Experiment 2 (N = 71) demonstrated that smiling male faces engendered an impression suitable for long-term partnership (e.g., high ratings of trustworthiness) while neutral faces produced an impression suitable for short-term partnership (e.g., high ratings of masculinity). We discuss these results in terms of opposing evolutionary strategies in mate choice: heritable benefit versus paternal investment.

18.
Shinrigaku Kenkyu ; 85(1): 60-8, 2014 Apr.
Artículo en Japonés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24804431

RESUMEN

The Operation Span Test (OSPAN) is widely used to assess working memory capacity. However, this instrument has been rarely used to test Japanese participants because its task was not sufficiently difficult. The mean score for the original computerized OSPAN often reached a ceiling when Japanese participants were tested. In this study, we developed a computerized version of OSPAN for Japanese participants by increasing the task difficulty of the arithmetic procedures. The OSPAN scores were normally distributed and the mean score was approximately 50%. There were positive correlations between OSPAN scores and other scores of working memory measurements, such as a reading span test and a digit span test. These results suggest that the Japanese OSPAN is a reliable and valid measurement of working memory to test Japanese participants.


Asunto(s)
Memoria a Corto Plazo , Pruebas Psicológicas , Pueblo Asiatico , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Adulto Joven
19.
Shinrigaku Kenkyu ; 85(1): 87-92, 2014 Apr.
Artículo en Japonés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24804434

RESUMEN

This study investigated the effect of interpersonal dependency on judgments of gaze direction of individuals with different facial expressions. Based on interpersonal dependency scores, 46 participants were divided into two groups (high interpersonal dependency and low interpersonal dependency). Participants judged the gaze direction of photographs of faces with angry, neutral or happy expressions. Relative to the low interpersonal dependency group, the high interpersonal dependency group was more accurate in the judgments of gaze direction. This tendency was more salient for the happy and neutral expressions than for the angry expressions. Since people with high interpersonal dependency are highly motivated to seek support from others, this result suggests that they are sensitive to signals with pro-social information such as the gaze direction of others with positive attitudes.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Oculares , Relaciones Interpersonales , Expresión Facial , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
20.
Shinrigaku Kenkyu ; 85(5): 474-81, 2014 Dec.
Artículo en Japonés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25639030

RESUMEN

Quantitative assessment of handedness is required in various clinical and research settings in psychology, neuroscience, and medicine. In the present study we tested the reliability and validity of a Japanese version of the FLANDERS handedness questionnaire, which was a new measure of skilled hand preference originally reported by Nicholls, Thomas, Loetscher, and Grimshaw (2013). Participants (N=431) completed three types of handedness questionnaires: the FLANDERS handedness questionnaire, Edinburgh Handedness Inventory, and H · N handedness test. Factor analysis revealed that the Japanese version of FLANDERS handedness questionnaire had a single-factor structure and high internal consistency. This questionnaire also posssed high test-retest reliability and criterion-referenced validity. These results indicate that the Japanese version of the FLANDERS handedness questionnaire is a valid and useful measure of skilled hand preference for Japanese participants.


Asunto(s)
Lateralidad Funcional , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Pueblo Asiatico , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...